|
MILESTONES AND
BREAKTHROUGHS: UNDERSTANDING LEARNING DIFFERENCES RESEARCH TO IMPROVE
TEACHING METHODS
|
Text
and Schematics by Hilda Coyne |
Educators of
learning different students may improve teaching methods by understanding
the neuroscientific bases of the differences, which need not be
a daunting task. From the first pioneer in learning difference neuroscientific
research, Dr. Samuel Torrey Orton, to the most recent, including
Dr. Sally Shaywitz, et al, scientists and educators have delineated
the etiological causes of and effective methods for the remediation
of these deficits. This article describes the research of a few
scientists, some of whose findings have application in teaching
students with learning differences.
To begin with,
neuroscience historians generally credit the French neurologist
Dr. Paul Broca with the inception of the modern study of language
impairments. His studies influenced popular opinion in the belief
that there was a language center in the brain. In 1863, he published
a paper describing eight cases in which damage to the frontal lobe
in the left hemisphere of the brain disturbed language expression,
determined from post-mortem studies. This region of the brain, now
called Broca's area, begins the analysis of phonemes, e.g., the
/k/ sound of the letter C in cat, and affects speech production.
In 1874, the
German neurologist Dr. Karl Wernecke reported that lesions in the
left hemisphere near the auditory cortex disrupt normal speech.
This region, now called Wernecke's area, facilitates language comprehension.
While Broca and Wernecke did not initiate research in dyslexia,
their studies proved beneficial to those who did that research.
Dr. Norman Geschwind, a neurologist at Harvard Medical School, later
pioneered in the development of the Wernecke-Geschwind Connectionist
Model, which increased the comprehension of the path of language
in the brain.
|
Fig. 1 The Wernecke-Geschwind Connectionist
Model
|
Auditory input
is received in the primary auditory cortex, but an individual may
not understand the word until processing of the signal occurs in
Wernecke's area. If an individual wishes to speak the word, a representation
transmits from Wernecke's area to Broca's area, which evokes a program
for articulation, supplied to the motor cortex, that then drives
the muscles to produce speech. For most individuals, these functional
regions are found only in the left hemisphere.
In 1896, Dr.
James Hinshelwood, an ophthalmologist in Scotland, introduced the
term congenital word blindness to describe patients with reading
disorders. He believed the problems they had with visual memory
related to reading derived from brain injury or defects in the left
language portion of the brain. Dr. Samuel Torrey Orton, however,
receives the credit for initiating neurological dyslexia research.
A psychiatrist and neuroscientist, he introduced the term strephosymbolia
from the Greek, meaning twisted symbols, to describe reading reversals.
While he predicted the precise location in the brain for the site
of the difference in brain architecture, he did not receive all
of the funding necessary to pursue that aspect of dyslexia research.
His associate, Anna Gillingham, a psychologist, developed the prototype
for multisensory instruction of dyslexic individuals.
Geschwind,
along with Dr. Albert Galaburda and several other doctors, discovered
differences in the brain architecture of dyslexic individuals as
compared to those of the non-dyslexic individuals. In 1979, Drs.
Galaburda and T.I. Kemper published their findings, reporting the
first biological evidence of the differences from post-mortem studies.
|
Fig. 2 The planum temporale
|
Please note
that in a transverse section of the brain in the planum temporale,
a part of the temporal lobe which is at the side of the brain, the
non-dyslexic brain is asymmetrical, whereas the dyslexic brain seems
symmetrical. Since these differences appear in one of the language
processing portions of the brain, this may indicate that these differences
in brain architecture might result in differences in processing
either receptive (reading/listening) or expressive (writing/speaking)
language.
Another
major discovery, the slower processing that dyslexic individuals employ
to process visual input, followed. Dr. Margaret Livingstone,
a neurobiologist at Harvard Medical School, reported that
dyslexic individuals exhibit some slow processing of visual input in
the mid-brain region, the first relay station for processing
visual input from the eye to the visual cortex (in the occipital
lobe near the back of the brain) which analyzes the message.
|
|
Fig. 3. The visual pathway
|
|
Within the
mid-brain region, the left and right sides of the thalamus each
contains two nuclei. The layers of the nuclei lie in a bent shape,
which explains their designation as geniculate, from the Latin geniculatus
which means bent like a knee. The lateral geniculate nuclei (LGN),
near the sides of the brain, process visual input, and the medial
geniculate nuclei (MGN), near the middle of the brain, process auditory
input.
|
|
Fig. 4 The left and right sides of the thalamus |
Fig. 5. The lateral and medial geniculate nuclei |
Within
each geniculate nucleus there is a parvocellular system (parvo
meaning small), and a magnocellular system (magno meaning
large). The parvo cells (X) carry out slower processes for
perceiving still images, color detail and high contrasts,
and the magno cells (O) carry out fast processes for perceiving
position, motion, shape and low contrast.
|
|
Fig. 6 The parvocellular and magnocellular
systems
|
|
The scientists
found that the magno cells in dyslexic individuals were smaller than those
in non-dyslexic individuals, and that the dyslexic individuals processed low contrast
information more slowly than non-dyslexic individuals.
Next, Dr. Alexander
Pollatsek reported important findings. Eye movements, called saccades,
allow non-dyslexic readers to fixate on an image so that the brain
perceives it before advancing to the next image. The image during
the saccade is suppressed so as not to overlap the next image. The
sustained or parvocellular system operates during fixation and the
transient or magnocellular system operates during saccades or movements
to the next fixation. Dyslexic individuals have a deficit in saccadic suppression.
Geneticists
also have provided relevant and significant data, and have documented
the heritability of dyslexia with DNA testing. Dr. Jeffrey Gilger
states that suspect genes are thought to lie on chromosomes 2, 3,
6, 7, l5, 18, and perhaps others. This heritability has implications
for diagnosis and counseling of family members, since not every
member is affected, and those who are affected may not exhibit identical
manifestations of dyslexia.
Most recently,
Dr. Sally Shaywitz, et al, at Yale University Medical School, reported
the results of a longitudinal study. The researchers tested the
cohort in reading performance in elementary and secondary schools
yearly. The researchers then categorized the readers as non-impaired
(NI) who scored well in both schools, as accuracy improved readers
(AIR) who scored poorly in elementary school but improved in secondary
school, and as persistently poor readers (PPR) who scored poorly
consistently.
The neuroscientists
assigned the readers two primary tasks: reading real words to match
them by category (CAT), and reading pseudowords to see if they rhyme
(NWR), then scanned the brains during these tasks. The first row
of abbreviations indicates the tasks administered, NWR or CAT, and
the next row identifies the type of student, NI, AIR or PPR. The
columns contain superimpositions of the scans of all participants
within each group, in transverse sections nearest the top, middle
or bottom of the brain. The data within the scans include red and
yellow patches, which designate greater activation.
|
Fig. 7 Brain activation model
|
Some terms
useful in reading the original text including the brains scans (please
see the bibliography) follow. The variable n indicates the number
of participants in each group of readers. A pixel is equal to the
dot in a photograph, e.g., in a newspaper photograph, and a voxel
is a three dimensional pixel. The term seed as applied in reading
a brain scan refers to the starting point from which measurement
of other data is possible. Functional connectivity is a term that represents which areas are correlated with similar activity.
The neuroscientists
discovered that the PPR participants processed information differently
from the other participants. The
researchers analyzed the functional connectivity in the real word
reading category (CAT) most since they found that the PPR participants
demonstrated unexpected brain activation during that task. According to Shaywitz, the researchers
hypothesize that in NI readers the occipitotemporal region processes
print in a linguistically structured manner, and should interact
with other areas involved in orthographic and phonological processing.
It is also hypothesized that in the PPR participants the occipitotemporal
area serves as a visually based memory system and should interact
with other areas involved in memory retrieval.
The ensuing
scans within that article contrast the results within each group with one another. In
addition, the researchers tested the I.Q. of the participants in
the study, and found that the NI and AIR participants scored higher
in elementary and secondary schools than the PPR participants. The
researchers concluded that the AIR and PPR groups began school with
similar abilities, but not necessarily the same deficit. The AIR group had better cognitive abilities
and advanced in reading ability because of that cognition. Is it
possible that the PPR participants, from lower socioeconomic strata
and attending more subsidized lunch schools with generally fewer
amenities had less opportunity for auditory stimuli such as music
instruction, and thus had more unremediated auditory processing
deficits which prevented them from using the occipitotemporal area
except as a primarily visual system? Additionally, is it further
possible that, if they had unremediated auditory processing deficits,
they scored lower on I.Q. tests overall because of the portions
administered verbally? Furthermore, since the parents of the PPR
participants more often came from a lower socioeconomic strata than
the parents of the NI and AIR participants, is it possible that
more parents of the PPR participants read poorly or not at all,
or held two or more jobs? This lessens the chances of their reading
to their children or of interacting with them in other ways that
develop language skills such as communication at mealtimes or trips
to the library or museum.
Most important,
since the PPR participants attended schools where a higher percent
received school-subsidized meals than either the NI or AIR participants,
the PPR participants may have resided in areas with a higher degree
of poverty and thus less of a tax base to fund academic services
than the other readers. This, in turn, may indicate that the schools
the PPR participants attended not only had fewer amenities such
as music, art, and library services, the school personnel likely
faced severely diminished operating budgets. As a result, the PPR
participants may have had less textbooks, generally outdated and
in disrepair, or not any, less heat or none in winter, fewer and
less experienced teachers, far larger class sizes in schools in
need of repair, some rooms sealed off, and more discipline
problems and violence, any or all of which may contribute to low
scores in standardized testing.
Lastly, while some note it is difficult at times to distinguish want
of effort from impairment, others observe a fear of failure along with a learning difference produces want of effort. However, the
appropriate intervention may facilitate comprehension and subsequently
improve perseverance and performance. The authors stress the need for
an increase in phonics and vocabulary instruction in order to decode
and comprehend that which students read. Thus, this remarkable research
may lead to better instruction and greater funding for deserving
students.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Baker,
D., Ed. (1984). Brain Research Update. Perspectives on Dyslexia,
Aug., 1-4.
Bear,
M., Connors, B. and Paradiso, M. (1996). Neuroscience: Exploring
the Brain. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.
Birch,
H.G. and Belmont, L. (1964). Auditory-visual integration in normal
and retarded Readers, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 34: 852-61.
Blakeslee,
S.S. (1991). Study Ties Dyslexia to Brain Flaw Affecting Vision
and Other Senses, New York Times, 15 Sept. 1.1.
Clark,
D.B. and Uhry, J.K. (1995). Dyslexia: Theory & Practice of Remedial
Instruction. Parkton, MD: York Press.
Dykman,
R.A. and Ackerman, P.T. (1991). Attention deficit disorder and specific
reading disability, separate but often overlapping disorders. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 24:96-103.
Galaburda,
A.M. and Kemper, T.I. (1979). Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in
developmental dyslexia. A case study. Annals of Neurology, 6: 94-100.
Galaburda,
A.M., Ed. (1993). Dyslexia and Development: Neurobiological Aspects
of Extra-Ordinary Brains, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Geschwind,
N. (1982). Why Orton Was Right. Annals of Dyslexia. 32:13-30.
Gilger,
J. (2003). Genes and Dyslexia. Perspectives, 29 (2): 6-8.
Gorman,
C. (2003). The New Science of Dyslexia. Time Magazine. July 18,
52-58.
Hinshelwood,
J. (1917. Congenital Word Blindness. London: H.K. Lewis.
Livingstone,
M. and Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of Form, Color, Movement and
Depth: Anatomy, Physiology and Perception. Science 240:740-749.
Lovegrove,
W.J., and Williams, M. J. (1993). Visual temporal processing deficits
in specific reading disability. Visual Processes in Reading and
Reading Disabilities, D. M. Willows, R.S. Kruk and E. Corcos. (Eds.)
Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Money,
J. and Schiffman, G. (Eds.) (1966). The Disabled Reader, Baltimore,
MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Orton,
S.T. (1928). Specific reading disability strephosymbolia. The
Journal of the American Medical Association, 90-1095-9.
Pollatsek,
A. (1993). Eye movements in reading. Visual Processes in Reading
and Reading Disabiities, D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, and H. Corcos.
(Eds.). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rosenzweig,
M., Leiman, A. and Breedlove, S.M. (1999). Biological Psychology:
An Introduction to Behavioral, Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience,
Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.
Shaywitz,
S., et al. (2003). Neural Systems for Compensation and Persistence:
Young Adult Outcome of Childhood Reading Disability, Journal of
Biological Psychiatry, 54: 25-33.
|